Education, tips and tricks to help you conduct better fMRI experiments.
Sure, you can try to fix it during data processing, but you're usually better off fixing the acquisition!

Monday, December 8, 2014

Concomitant physiologic changes as potential confounds for BOLD-based fMRI: a checklist


Many thanks for all the feedback on the draft version of this post.

Main updates since the draft:
  • Added DRIFTER to the list of de-noising methods
  • Added a reference for sex differences in hematocrit and the effects on BOLD
  • Added several medication classes, including statins, sedatives & anti-depressants
  • Added a few dietary supplements, under Food

Please do continue to let me know about errors and omissions, especially new papers that get published. I'll gladly do future updates to this post.


UPDATES:

(Since this post release on 8th Dec, 2014.) 

17th Dec 2014: Update for cortisol, highlighted in yellow.
18th Dec 2014: Update for methylphenidate, atomoxetine & amphetamine, highlighted in orange.
19th Dec 2014: Update for oxytocin, highlighted in turquoise.
13th Jan 2015: Update for effects of the scanner itself, highlighted in green.
27th Feb 2015: Added a new reference, hematocrit effects on resting-state fMRI.
27th May 2016: Added new references on altitude, sleep, pharmacological fMRI (with morphine & alcohol).
1st Feb & 2nd Mar 2017: Added new references for flavonoids in foods, highlighted in red.
23rd May 2017: Added a new reference for the effect of carbon monoxide on BOLD.
______________________



A recent conversation on Twitter led to the suggestion that someone compile a list of physiological effects of concern for BOLD. That is, a list of potentially confounding physiological changes that could arise sympathetically in an fMRI experiment, such as altered heart rate due to the stress of a task, or that could exist as a systematic difference between groups. What follows is the result of a PubMed literature search (mostly just the abstracts) where I have tried to identify either recent review articles or original research that can be used as starting points for learning more about candidate effects. Hopefully you can then determine whether a particular factor might be of concern for your experiment.

This is definitely not a comprehensive list of all literature pertaining to all potential physiological confounds in fMRI, and I apologize if your very important contribution didn't make it into the post. Also, please note that I am not a physiologist so if I go seriously off piste in interpreting the literature, please forgive me and then correct my course. I would like to hear from you (comments below, or via Twitter) if I have omitted critical references or effects from the list, or if I have misinterpreted something. As far as possible I've tried to restrict the review to work in humans unless there was nothing appropriate, in which case I've included some animal studies if I think they are directly relevant. I'll try to keep this post up-to-date as new studies come out and as people let me know about papers I've missed.

A final caution before we begin. It occurs to me that some people will take this list as (further) proof that all fMRI experiments are hopelessly flawed and will use it as ammunition. At the other extreme there will be people who see this list as baseless scare-mongering. How you use the list is entirely up to you, but my intent is to provide cautious fMRI scientists with a mechanism to (re)consider potential physiologic confounds in their experiments, and perhaps stimulate the collection of parallel data that might add power to those experiments.


Getting into BOLD physiology


There are some good recent articles that introduce the physiological artifacts of prime concern. Tom Liu has reviewed neurovascular factors in resting-state functional MRI and shows how detectable BOLD signals arise from physiological changes in the first place. Kevin Murphy et al. then review some of the most common confounds in resting-state fMRI and cover a few ways these spurious signal changes can be characterized and even removed from data. Finally, Dan Handwerker et al. consider some of the factors causing hemodynamic variations within and, in particular, between subjects.

Once you start really looking into this stuff it can be hard not to get despondent. Think of the large number of potential manipulations as opportunities, not obstacles! Perhaps let The Magnetic Fields get you in the mood with their song, "I don't like your (vascular) tone." Then read on. It's a long list.


Breathing and heart rates


These are undoubtedly the two big issues and will be top-of-mind for most people. Murphy et al. do a good job introducing the topic of cleaning raw data, e.g. using independent respiration and heart rate data. There are numerous correction methods available today (see the section, "Time series cleanup with physiological recordings" in the Murphy paper) and these references are perhaps the best places to start if you want to dig into the origins of the problems. In reverse chronological order we have:

PHYCAA+ by Churchill & Strother
GLMdenoise by Kay, Rokem, Winawer, Dougherty & Wandell.
RIPTiDe by Frederick, Nickerson & Tong (using near-infrared spectroscopy, NIRS)
DRIFTER by Särkkä, Solin, Nummenmaa, Vehtari, Auranen, Vanni & Lin.
RVT-HR by Chang, Cunningham & Glover
RVT by Birn, Diamond, Smith & Bandettini
RETROICOR by Glover, Li & Reese

Then there are also numerous ICA-based methods, including multi-echo acquisition with a model fit. I won't comment on the relative or absolute performance of any of these schemes, just let you know you have options! But one important lesson seems to be this: measuring heart rate and respiration independently isn't likely to hurt.

I'll also mention the inter-relatedness of HR and breathing, through vagal tone. This has implications for fMRI studies employing emotional stimuli.


CO2: hypercapnia and hypocapnia


Inspired CO2 is a well-known and powerful vasodilator. Similarly, breath holding is known to lead to hypercapnia (increases in arterial CO2) and thus vasodilation. Thus, under normal conditions (no external CO2 source) the arterial concentration of CO2 will be closely linked to the breathing rate, as demonstrated in this 2004 study. The quantitative relationship between breathing rate and end-tidal CO2 is further investigated in this 2009 study.

As for dealing with the variation, in a 2012 study it was demonstrated that clamping arterial CO2 to a narrow range improved retrospective physiological artifact removal from resting-state and task-based data. I like this idea because I've often wondered whether not having the magnet bore properly ventilated could cause hypercapnia. I can't find any papers that have looked at this issue, however. The closest I've found is a study that used CO2 supplied artificially, clearly demonstrating the potential interaction of inspired CO2 with stimulus-driven BOLD. (There was an earlier study in rats that reached essentially the same conclusion.) It's probably best to keep the ventilation constant in the magnet bore for all subjects, just in case.

It turns out there may be gender bias, too. A 2010 study found that men have higher cerebrovascular reactivity to a CO2 challenge. So, not only do men tend to exhibit more head movement than women on average, men also appear to be more sensitive to CO2.


O2: hyperoxia and hypoxia


While refluxed CO2 may be a legitimate concern, incidental hyperoxia seems unlikely unless your MRI facility has a very strange design indeed. But, if you happen to be doing fMRI of divers, high-altitude pilots or astronauts in a simulated environment, do bear in mind the possibility of arterial BOLD from dissolved O2 if your subjects are breathing 100% oxygen. I suppose this ought to be a concern in anesthetized fMRI subjects, too. (I'll consider the physiologic consequences of anesthetics below.) If you're comparing normal volunteers (awake) to anesthetized patients then you might want to consider giving the former group the same gas mixture (sans anesthetic) as the latter.

What about hypoxia? If you have a well-designed MRI facility then ordinarily acute hypoxia shouldn't be an issue. But if your scanner is in a poorly ventilated basement you might want to consider an oxygen sensor, assuming you don't already have one, or improving your air conditioning. Otherwise you may be contributing to the variability in your subjects' BOLD responses.

Chronic hypoxia may be an important issue to track, too. Yan et al. studied immigrants who had grown up at high altitude (HA, 2616–4200 m) and relocated to sea level (SL, < 400 m) to a group of sea-level controls, and found both anatomical and resting state fMRI differences associated with "chronic high altitude hypoxia." These changes were in spite of "...no significant differences in hemoglobin levels, circulating red blood cell count, blood pressure, and pulse rate between HA residents and SL controls." In a separate study, Yan et al. found persistent, correlated alterations in verbal working memory and BOLD responses for subjects who had relocated from HA (2,982.8 ± 478.7 m) to SL (< 400 m), even though they had been resident at sea level for at least one year and in spite of consistent hemoglobin levels.

If you live in a part of the world where your subjects might be spending their weekends in high mountains, bear in mind that even relatively short stays at extreme altitude may have an effect. Villien et al. observed that a 6-day stay at 4350m produced increased regional CBF measurable six hours after subjects returned to sea level.


Vasomotion


According to that great oracle, Wikipedia, "vasomotion is the spontaneous oscillation in tone of blood vessels, independent of heart beat, innervation or respiration." Fair enough. Our concern is whether vasomotion might differ across groups, or perhaps might even vary for an individual under different conditions. Again according to Wikipedia, "vasomotion has been shown to be altered in a variety of pathological situations, with vessels from both hypertensive and diabetic patients displaying altered flow patterns as compared to normotensive vessels."

Murphy et al. say this: "If vasomotion is independent of cardiac, respiratory, arterial CO2 concentration and blood pressure fluctuations, its low-frequency characteristics will present another confound for resting-state fMRI BOLD oscillations."

How serious a concern might spontaneous low-frequency variations be for your average BOLD study? A recent paper by Tong & Frederick claims that low frequency oscillations (LFOs) are a larger component of physiologic noise than respiration or heart rate, even when respiration and heart rate frequencies are aliased because of the relatively long TRs that are typical of most fMRI studies. There is also evidence from intraoperative recordings that vasomotion (at ~0.1 Hz) is "spatially localized to distinct regions of the cortex, exhibited wave-like propagation, and involved oscillations in the diameter of specific pial arterioles, indicating that the effect was not the result of systemic blood pressure oscillations." Not small potatoes, then.

What do these LFOs tell us, and (how) do they relate to the ongoing intrinsic neural activity that we assume is driving resting-state fMRI? Methinks this might become the next fashionable study area, if it's not already.


Blood pressure


A good introduction to blood pressure (BP) and its relationship to CBF is given in the Murphy, Birn & Bandettini review. We might be interested in the subject's baseline (pre-scan) BP, but we might also be interested in the BP during a time series acquisition given that BP changes when laying supine versus standing, during stress, etc.

I've been unable to locate any papers showing whether TR-to-TR (real-time) BP is of any use in explaining variance in BOLD time series data. Baseline BP (before MRI) was found to offer only a small normalizing effect on visual-evoked BOLD signals when tested across two conditions; baseline venous T2 was considerably better. A study by Lui et al. in cocaine-dependent (human) subjects found that dobutamine infusion raised mean arterial BP (MAP) but produced only localized BOLD signal changes (in anterior cingulate) that correlated with the BP rise. Gianaros et al. observed a correlation between mean arterial BP and BOLD activity in several brain regions of participants conducting a stressful Stroop task, with BP measured once for each block of sixteen 90-second task blocks.

This is Murphy et al.'s take:
"Evidence of the influence of blood pressure oscillations on resting-state fMRI fluctuations in humans is sparse. Blood pressure levels in rats have been shown to affect evoked fMRI responses, with transient hypertension increasing BOLD (Wang et al., 2006) and CBF (Qiao et al., 2007) signals. Under hypotension, neural activity-evoked CBV increases in visual cortex are negligible compared to ~ 10% at normal blood pressure levels (Nagaoka et al., 2006). Increases in the amplitude of low-frequency BOLD fluctuations have been demonstrated with a drop in mean arterial pressure (Biswal and Kannurpatti, 2009). As supporting evidence in humans, BOLD signal correlates of heart rate and pulse height in the low frequency range have been discovered with fluctuations in cardiac rate explaining up to 11% of the variance in the resting-state BOLD signal (Chang et al., 2009, de Munck et al., 2008 and Shmueli et al., 2007)."

What about chronic hypertension? Another study from Gianaros et al. used the Stroop test to stress healthy young volunteers as a model for determining whether reactivity to psychological stressors might be used to assess risk from hypertension. They found: "Individuals exhibiting greater stressor-evoked MAP reactivity showed (1) greater amygdala activation, (2) lower amygdala gray matter volume, and (3) stronger positive functional connectivity between the amygdala and perigenual anterior cingulate cortex and brainstem pons."

Hypotension may be a factor in cases of major blood loss, perhaps including recent blood donation. While I wasn't able to find any papers looking at recent blood loss (or donation) on BOLD fMRI, Kalisch et al. used a hemorrhage model in rats and observed heterogeneous correlations between BOLD and BP. They suggest that "...a BOLD decrease during a decrease in BP may result from an increase in mostly venous cerebral blood volume (CBV) as an autoregulatory response to maintain cerebral blood flow (CBF) during decreased perfusion pressure."


Hematocrit level


Hematocrit level is the percentage of red blood cells in the blood. And since red blood cells carry hemoglobin, which itself transports the usable oxygen in blood, a person's hematocrit level is an important parameter when considering BOLD signal variations. Hematocrit levels could be unusually high in endurance athletes or folks who have just returned from an extended time living at altitude, for example. At the other extreme, we would be concerned about fMRI subjects experiencing anemia.

Levin et al. found a positive linear dependence of BOLD percent activation (BPA) on hematocrit level, the relationship being stronger in men than in women. Furthermore, "...9 men were studied before and following isotonic saline hemodilution, resulting in an average 6% reduction in hematocrit and an 8-31% reduction in BPA." This suggests checking whether your subject has just come off an i.v. saline drip because of food poisoning or flu.

There are also well-known sex differences for hematocrit level, with men having an average value several percent (5% seems to be the consensus figure) higher than women. A poster by Yang et al. at this year's HBM conference, now published in this paper, showed how variation in hematocrit across individuals produced a systematic difference in several measures derived from resting-state fMRI.

Most causes of anemia are chronic and presumably you would be aware of them, but there are forms of anemia that might be of issue for BOLD studies in so-called normal volunteers. Recent utilization of i.v. fluids has just been mentioned while recent blood loss, perhaps from major surgery or giving blood, were considered in a prior section. Are these important? Knowing about them might permit you to interpret an outlier. Low hematocrit could also be relevant to studies of certain patient groups, e.g. acute traumatic brain injury, if polytrauma could have resulted in recent loss of blood.

Possible variations in hemoglobin suggests a role for normalization when comparing across groups (or even within subjects over an extended period of time). Lu, Yezhuvath & Xiao investigated the utility of baseline venous oxygenation as a normalizing parameter, finding that "...visual-evoked BOLD signal is significantly correlated with baseline venous T2 (P = 0.0003) and inclusion of physiologic modulator in the regression analysis can substantially reduce P values of group-level statistical tests."

I'll include here a special case. Sickle cell anemia (SCA) has profound effects on the BOLD response because resting CBF is increased. Remarkably, Bob Ogg's group was able to get fMRI responses in SCA children but the BOLD signals were diminished in amplitude. What is especially intriguing is that "... blood hemoglobin concentration and resting CBF were not predictive of BOLD signal amplitude in the SCA patients." No attempt was made to measure venous blood T2 in this particular study. Definitely an area ripe for more research.


Exercise


The long-term effects of exercise, particularly in endurance athletes, should be reflected in hematocrit level. What about the short-term effects of recent exercise on BOLD? If your subjects arrive at the scanner having just come from a boot camp class at the gym, or they've ridden a bike twenty miles from home, do you need to know about it?

A study by MacIntosh et al. showed that twenty minutes of aerobic exercise less than an hour before scanning decreased CBF in grey matter for up to 40 minutes. BOLD results from a go/no-go attention task were mostly consistent with the pre-exercise baseline. However, the same group used a similar exercise regimen prior to acquiring resting-state fMRI and "...observed a change in the resting-state BOLD functional connectivity of young healthy adults in three [resting state networks] RSNs, predominantly localized to cortical areas involved in sensorimotor activity." The latter study was limited in that "...at the time of the repeat rs-fMRI scan, the heart rate decreased after exercise but was nonetheless still significantly greater than the pre-exercise heart rate. Using heart rate as a covariate in the paired design group analysis did influence the session-related findings for two of the three significant RSNs. Blood pressure was not measured continuously so we cannot rule out the possibility that it too was elevated at the time of the repeat rs-fMRI scan."

There are clear opportunities for more research in this direction, but it would appear that we should insist on baseline measures of hematocrit (or venous T2) and blood pressure in addition to heart rate and respiration, or we risk misinterpreting BOLD changes.


(De)hydration


Using thermal exercise so that subjects dehydrated via sweating, versus a non-thermal exercise control, Kempton et al. found an increased fronto-parietal BOLD response during an executive function task while cognitive performance and CBF were unchanged. The authors suggest: "This pattern indicates that participants exerted a higher level of neuronal activity in order to achieve the same performance level." 


Caffeine


The effect of this vasoconstrictor on BOLD seem to have been studied more than any other drug (or foodstuff), probably because it's the most widely used stimulant in the world. There are many references to choose from, and what emerges is a picture best described as "it's complicated."

For a start it turns out that a subject's normal caffeine usage makes a difference if they're given a caffeine challenge. Laurienti et al. found "...that the BOLD signal change in visual cortex was significantly greater in high users than in low users in the presence of caffeine." Are your subjects high or low users? Would they even know? Here's a searchable website containing many of the common dietary sources of caffeine. Many sources are well-known and obvious, e.g. dark chocolate. Some, though, may surprise you.

Next there is the dose response. The amount of caffeine alters BOLD magnitude non-linearly. According to Chen & Parrish, the greatest effects on BOLD are associated with an intermediate caffeine dose of 2.5 mg/kg. (It must only be the neural response that goes all jittery with high doses!) In the early 2000s it was even suggested to use caffeine as a BOLD signal booster, but the non-linear dose response seems to have nixed that idea. Laurienti et al. puts it this way: "It is not possible to consistently enhance BOLD signal intensity magnitude by decreasing resting perfusion with caffeine."

In case the picture isn't sufficiently complicated already, it has been found that caffeine alters the temporal dynamics of visual BOLD responses. Intriguingly, administration of caffeine has also been found to enhance the linearity of the BOLD response to rapid visual stimuli. One assumes there must be a dose response that hasn't yet been investigated, however. What about resting-state fMRI and caffeine? For starters, caffeine changes resting-state connectivity for motor cortex. It also increases the variability of motor cortical connectivity.

So it appears that we may have an additional, potentially large, source of inter- (and intra?) subject variability based on the pattern of normal caffeine use, right? Curiously, the answer is ambiguous. Addicott, Peiffer & Laurienti investigated the effects of a caffeine dose (or placebo) on subjects representing a range of usage levels (including abstinence). Acute caffeine administration did produce measurable effects in BOLD, consistent with prior reports. But the changes weren't moderated either by normal use or by abstinence in regular users. The authors conclude "...that dietary caffeine use does not produce a significant effect on task-related BOLD activation."

Why might this be? Griffeth, Perthen & Buxton used a quantitative fMRI experiment to investigate caffeine's effects on BOLD and CBF simultaneously. They observed offsetting changes in baseline blood flow and oxygen metabolism with subsequent responses to visual stimuli, such that: "The combined effect was that BOLD responses pre- and post-caffeine were similar despite large underlying physiological changes, indicating that the magnitude of the BOLD response alone should not be interpreted as a direct measure of underlying neurophysiological changes." 

Finally, let's assume you're interested in the neural consequences of caffeine use. How to differentiate from physiology? A good place to start is this 2010 review of caffeine's effects on cognition as well as BOLD. Then check out these more recent studies using simultaneous EEG-fMRI, comparing MEG to fMRI in the same subjects across separate sessions, or comparing fMRI to PET.

So, what's the bottom line regarding dietary caffeine as a potential BOLD confound? I don't know! But it sure looks like something that ought to be tracked, even if it's just self-reported accounts.

Consuming a stimulant in the land of depressants.


Alcohol


Ahhh, the world's favorite depressant. (The bulk of this post is being written overlooking vineyards in the Bennett Valley of Sonoma county, CA. Yes, that is coffee on the table above. Wine comes later, once the shadows point to bottom-right.) Let's deal with what we do know first. Acute administration of alcohol reduces BOLD activation in response to a visual stimulus, suppresses BOLD activity during a goal-directed visuomotor task, changes CBF in a dose-dependent manner, and modulates neurovascular coupling. Furthermore, acute alcohol consumption changes significantly several cognitive and visual networks mapped with a resting fMRI paradigm. (Also this study, on resting networks.) Finally, there are studies assessing the chronic effects of alcohol abuse, but I don't think chronic alcohol consumption can be considered as a potential physiologic confound because the neural effects will likely dominate.

Very surprisingly, nobody seems to have looked at the consequences of hangovers on fMRI. If acute alcohol administration causes changes to physiology and these changes are dose-dependent then at what point may we consider them negated? An hour? Eight hours? A night's sleep? Surely - surely? - someone must have looked at this. If not, it's a race to see who can get the first study reported.


Nicotine


The literature on nicotine, and cigarette smoking in particular, is a mixed bag as far as potential confounds go. In a study on nicotine-dependent smokers, Jacobsen et al. found no effect of intravenous nicotine on BOLD signals produced by visual stimulation. However, citing the "Considerable variability across individuals... in both the behavioral and fMRI blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response to nicotine" that had been found in prior studies, Warbrick et al. observed: "...some participants showed an increase in activation while others showed a decrease in BOLD activation from the placebo to the [nasal] nicotine condition." Nineteen of the 41 total subjects were smokers.

So much for the controlled administration of nicotine in an experiment. What about the effects of smoking itself? Smoking may be a much larger concern because cigarettes produce carbon monoxide (CO), and hemoglobin has a high affinity for CO. If hemoglobin is carrying CO then it can't be carrying O2. How much of a concern is this displacement for BOLD? The literature doesn't say. Inhaling cigarette smoke is associated with a high number of health issues, including reduced pulmonary function. but I didn't find any literature addressing these concerns for BOLD-based fMRI.  Carbon monoxide at a typical level associated with cigarette smoking (6 ppm exhaled) has been shown to decrease significantly the global response to hypercapnia, in subjects who had never smoked (Bendell et al.). However, CO had variable spatial effects in task fMRI, increasing BOLD signal in visual cortex while decreasing it in premotor areas, for a visual-motor paradigm. The acute effects of CO on CBF or BOLD signal in habitual or casual smokers or former smokers has yet to be studied.

One study from 2008 looked at heavy smoking as a potential confound in BOLD studies of schizophrenia, but found no difference with non-smokers using a sensorimotor task. However, another study from 2008 found significant differences in breath hold and visual activation tasks for heavy smokers versus controls. Why the contradiction? No idea I'm afraid.

Given the well documented respiratory and cardiac issues associated with chronic cigarette smoking, it seems to me that there is a very strong likelihood of systematic bias in the physiology of any group of smokers versus non-smokers. Would we compare marathon runners to couch potatoes and not expect significant differences in physiology? Surely, then, we should insist on some baseline measures of physiology, including perhaps pulmonary function tests (e.g. spirometry), or we should expect high inter-subject variability as well as smoker versus non-smoker group differences.

The physiologic effects produced by other nicotine delivery methods such as chewing tobacco, e-cigarettes (vaporizers) or patches may differ from the effects produced by cigarette smoking. Certainly that is my intuition but I wasn't able to locate any literature dealing with these issues. I think it suffices to say that we would want to uses caution when interpreting BOLD signals from smokers, and to predict systematic differences with non-smokers. That would just leave the dose effect (number of cigarettes/day, and perhaps depth and duration of smoke inhalation) to add a few more complications to the picture.


Illicit drugs


With the possible exception of cannabis (marijuana), we can probably discount most illegal (or partially legal) drugs as serious confounds for routine fMRI studies because they aren't in widespread use in the general population. (See Note 1.) But just in case, I did searches for several drugs, including ecstasy (MDMA), LSD and psychedelics in general, heroin, and methamphetamine, looking for direct investigations into possible confounding physiologic changes. Pattinson et al. studied the mu-opioid agonist remifentanil, finding only some small, regional modulations of the BOLD response to a hypercapnic challenge. Gollub et al. found that cocaine decreased CBF but BOLD responses to visual stimuli were normal. This doesn't imply that the underlying physiology is normal, however. As with caffeine, simultaneous changes in baseline CBF and oxygen metabolism could yield normal-looking responses in spite of different physiologic mechanisms. 

Cannabis is now in widespread use in most western populations. There are numerous studies of the (neural and behavioral) effects of this compound, but only a small handful of investigations of the physiologic effects relevant to BOLD.

There are a couple of literature reviews that have looked at neuroimaging and cannabis use. In a 2010 review the authors noted the general finding "...that resting global and prefrontal blood flow are lower in cannabis users than in controls." An updated review, in 2013, assessed the findings as suggesting "...different patterns of resting global and brain activity during the performance of several cognitive tasks both in adolescents and adults, which may indicate compensatory effects in response to chronic cannabis exposure." Only neuroimaging studies involving chronic cannabis users were considered. Both reviews highlighted the methodological limitations of the work conducted to date and the considerable heterogeneity of results.

What sort of physiologic confounds might be of concern in cannabis users, anyway? Let's look at the acute effects of smoking marijuana. O'Leary et al. used O-15 PET and found: "Smoking marijuana resulted in intoxication, as assessed by a behavioral rating scale, but did not significantly alter mean behavioral performance on the attention task. Heart rate and blood pressure increased dramatically following smoking of marijuana but not placebo cigarettes. However, mean global CBF did not change significantly. Increased rCBF was observed in orbital and mesial frontal lobes, insula, temporal poles, anterior cingulate, as well as in the cerebellum. The increases in rCBF in anterior brain regions were predominantly in "paralimbic" regions and may be related to marijuana's mood-related effects. Reduced rCBF was observed in temporal lobe auditory regions, in visual cortex, and in brain regions that may be part of an attentional network (parietal lobe, frontal lobe and thalamus)."

That there are immediate effects on physiology following cannabis administration isn't all that surprising. And, as with acute alcohol administration, subjects smoking a joint immediately before doing an fMRI experiment probably aren't the prime concern. (If they are, you might want to check the effects of carbon monoxide, as for regular cigarettes.) What about the hangover effects or the chronic effects on pulmonary function?

In a 2006 study, Sneider et al. obtained dynamic susceptibility contrast (DSC) MRI (i.e. gadolinium bolus) from twelve "current, long-term daily cannabis users between 6 and 36 hr after the subjects' last reported cannabis use. Cannabis users demonstrated significantly increased blood volumes in the right frontal area (p < .05), in the left temporal area (p < .005), and in the cerebellum (p < .005) relative to comparison subjects." They followed that study with a longer duration of abstinence, scanning subjects with DSC MRI at 7 and 28 days after last cannabis use: "The present findings demonstrate that at Day 7, cannabis users continued to display increased blood volumes in the right frontal region, the left and right temporal regions, and the cerebellum. However, after 28 days of abstinence, only the left temporal area and cerebellum showed significantly increased CBV values in cannabis users. These findings suggest that while CBV levels begin to normalize with continued abstinence from cannabis, specifically in frontal areas, other temporal and cerebellar brain regions show slower CBV decreases."

Acute differences in regional CBF were also found in adolescent marijuana users, but the differences had resolved after four weeks of monitored abstinence. CBF wasn't measured at intermediate times, however, so we can't tell from this study how long any physiologic hangover effects might last. It also isn't clear whether the persistent effects are neural, physiologic or both, but you'd think that anything lasting more than a few days would have a strong neural basis. (This 2010 study observed BOLD signal changes in a spatial working memory task dependent on recency of use in adolescents.)

But do changes in baseline CBV and CBF imply different neurovascular coupling or altered BOLD responses? These might be more problematic for interpretation of fMRI data than persistent (neural) effects. According to Murphy et al., perhaps not. They used a finger-tapping task and determined no differences between groups of users of cocaine, nicotine or cannabis and control subjects.


Medications


Many legal (prescription or over-the-counter) analgesic and anti-inflammatory drugs don't seem to have been studied for their effects on neurovascular coupling. I couldn't find anything on aspirin (acetylsalicylic acid), paracetamol (acetaminophen), ibuprofen, codeine or oxycodone (oxycontin).  I also couldn't find any references investigating the undesirable effects of Viagra (or its competitors) or antihistamines on BOLD physiology. (One group has even suggested using diphenhydramine, active ingredient in the US/Canadian version of the antihistamine drug, Benadryl as a way to combat nausea and vertigo in 7 T MRI.)

Sedatives have been used in several fMRI studies, but to date there have been no investigations into concomitant physiologic effects for the popular benzodiazepines such as diazepam (Valium), lorazepam (Ativan) or midazolam.  The non-benzodiazepine sedative zolpidem (Ambien) has demonstrated clear effects on visual BOLD signals with no concomitant change in heart rate or oxygen saturation. The same drug was shown to alter resting connectivity but the accompanying heart rate data were deemed unreliable due to equipment failures. I was unable to find any studies using another common non-benzodiazepine, eszopiclone (Lunesta).

Of those medications that have been considered for possible physiologic confounds, the non-steroidal anti-inflammatory indomethacin was studied a decade ago and found to change BOLD and CBF but not CMRO2 for subjects conducting a simple motor task.

Medicines aimed specifically at increasing a patient's blood flow are of obvious concern for fMRI. Acetazolamide (Diamox) was shown to increase resting cortical perfusion by 20% and decrease primary motor cortical BOLD activation by 35%, whereas the CBF response in primary motor cortex was unchanged in normal volunteers. A similar pattern was observed in patients with steno-occlusive coronary disease. However, another common vasodilator, glyceryl trinitrate, which increases resting cerebral blood volume (CBV), had no demonstrable effect on the BOLD response. The authors suggest that the glyceryl trinitrate affects large arteries only and has negligible effect on the microvascular system driving BOLD.

Blood pressure medications such as angiotensin, or anticoagulants such as warfarin, are presumably not something your subjects will use without telling you; your screening should catch these. Likewise asthma medications. I couldn't find any studies looking at potential physiologic confounds for these medications in any case. But Grichisch et al. investigated the potential effects of nasal insulin on CBF and BOLD and found no difference to a drug-free baseline. (Insulin and blood glucose level is considered again below, under Food.)

Statins (Lipitor, Zocor, Pravachol, etc.) appear to be in widespread and increasing use in western populations. These medicines are designed to reduce cholesterol synthesis in the liver, but there appear to be secondary outcomes with implications for cerebral hemodynamics, such as "...upregulation of endothelial nitric oxide synthase (eNOS) with a subsequent increase in nitric oxide (NO) bioavailability." Xu et al. investigated the hemodynamic consequences of taking atorvastatin (Lipitor) over four months for asymptomatic middle-aged adults. They observed numerous regional changes in BOLD, including both greater and faster regional responses, compared to a group receiving placebo. They observed no change in baseline CBF nor any change in mean transit time (MTT) of cerebral perfusion, suggesting that the regional BOLD changes came about through altered (improved) small vessel vasoreactivity.

Antidepressants, including several selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors (SSRIs), serotonin-norepinephrine re-uptake inhibitors (SNRIs), and a host of compounds belonging to other four-letter acronyms have been given to subjects in dozens of fMRI studies. But I was unable to find any systematic investigation of concomitant physiology, such as measuring CBF before and after drug administration.

And then there are the so-called "smart drugs" and other brain enhancers such as methylphenidate (Ritalin), amphetamine (Adderall) and their ilk. (If I'm not using the correct collective terms for these compounds, I apologize.) Rao et al. assessed BOLD and perfusion changes for subjects performing a finger tapping task before and after oral methylphenidate and found no changes in neurovascular coupling. Otherwise, I wasn't able to find much literature addressing the potential for physiologic confounds. Marquand et al. looked at CBF changes due to methylphenidate and atomoxetine (Strattera) while Nordin et al. assessed CBF changes due to amphetamine, both groups finding regional differences compared to placebo. But any implications for BOLD fMRI are unclear. Marquand et al. didn't record heart rate, respiration or blood pressure data whereas Nordin et al. found no differences in heart rate or respiration but did observe a small increase in blood pressure for amphetamine versus placebo. A 2012 study found "a small but significant impact on the cardiovascular system" after six months' use of methylphenidate and atomoxetine in Italian youths, including changes in heart rate (both compounds) and diastolic pressure (atomoxetine).

Literature coming out of the so-called pharmacological MRI (phMRI) field may provide the best clues for potential confounds to routine fMRI studies, should you have specific reason to be concerned about the use of medicines in your population of interest. Wang et al. reviewed the potential and challenges of using arterial spin labeling (ASL) in phMRI, "...with an emphasis on the methodologies used to control for potentially confounding vascular and systemic effects." A crossover study using morphine and alcohol by Khalili-Mahani et al. found: "Our analyses suggest a complex and spatially heterogeneous relation between RSNC [resting state network connectivity] and CBF across different functional networks and provide preliminary evidence that spatial distribution of neuroreceptors and cerebrovascular architecture play a role in dissociative relations between regional cerebral perfusion and functional connectivity across the brain."


Anesthetics


It's unlikely you'll be surprised by use of anesthetics among your subjects. But there is certainly interest in scanning people who may have had recent surgery, e.g. traumatic brain-injured patients who may have required surgery to treat other injuries or even the TBI itself. You would presumably want to discount any residual neural and physiologic effects of anesthesia. For example, Qui et al. found "Low-dose sevoflurane significantly altered the task-induced CBF-BOLD coupling." Post surgery one expects altered blood pressure, heart rate and breathing rate, so there are plenty of options to measure and control for concomitant physiologic effects.


Foods


It seems that the biggest potential physiological confounds lie with those dietary substances known to alter neural activity, as already reviewed. In this section I'll try to deal with latent effects from things your subjects ingest for other reasons. For sustenance, say. Before that, though, what about the possible effects of restricted nutrition?

There are a couple of fMRI studies on fasting, one that found an effect in a motor task and another that found altered functional connectivity of visual cortex. There are also several reports of altered BOLD signal in response to experimental hypoglycemia (induced via insulin administration), including cognitive, visual stimulation, and sentence comprehension fMRI tasks. All three studies observed regional differences when compared to normal glucose levels. (See Note 2.) Patients with type I diabetes also showed different BOLD activity compared to controls. Additionally, there is one study using CBF and measures of oxidative metabolism (CMRO2) to assess the effects of hypoglycemia. They found that "...metabolism and flow remained coupled. Elementary motor task activation was not associated with any consistent larger activated flows. Thus it remains that although mild hypoglycemia induced an increase in basal flow and metabolism, a similar increase was not seen in task activation." Finally, glucose and fructose infusions were compared to saline and found to alter BOLD signal magnitude in opposite directions.

These are interesting studies, but you're probably more interested in any possible effect of blood glucose level on normal BOLD variability when your subjects aren't fasting intentionally or having their blood sugar levels manipulated artificially. I couldn't find any literature addressing this specific concern, however there is a very recent paper that compared an overnight fast with normal and hyperglycemic conditions. While the paper's abstract makes it sound like there were big changes in BOLD, in the Discussion they write: "These effects are comparatively small, yet may interfere with design sensitivity, when fasting status or blood glucose is not controlled in fMRI experiments." I only skimmed the paper but was left with the impression that the jury is still out on this one. Hyperglycemia, specifically, doesn't seem to be a concern for BOLD. Gruetter et al. observed no significant effects on BOLD for blood glucose of up to 300% of control levels.

Now let's move to what your subjects might be eating before getting a scan. In 2003 it was reported that ingesting lipid - 50 ml of canola oil - decreased BOLD response in a finger tapping task. But the potential effect of triglyceride levels in blood was measured again in 2009 by another group, whereupon no significant effects on BOLD were observed. Why the contradiction? We can only speculate. The authors of the 2009 study offer several plausible explanations in their discussion, and I am inclined to go with the more recent result pending further experimental data.

Then there is the antithesis to the high fat diet: salad. Aamand et al. used a nitrate challenge, "corresponding to the nitrate content of a large plate of salad," to trigger changes in BOLD reactivity without altering the baseline CBF. CBF (with arterial spin labeling) in response to dietary nitrate was also measured by Presley et al. They found no global CBF changes but did observe some regional changes, in frontal lobe. The apparent contradiction with the first report was addressed by Aamand et al. as perhaps due to differences in sensitivity. In Aamand et al. they used only the inferior half of a 32-channel head coil which reduced the SNR in frontal lobe for that study.

Dietary supplements may deserve their own entire section at some point but for now I'll include them as food because they are generally unregulated, unlike FDA-controlled medications. Creatine, usually as the monohydrate, is a popular sports supplement that has been shown to decrease the magnitude of a visual BOLD response by 16%. I didn't find any literature assessing the effects of carnitine, calcium, glycerol, vitamin A (including retinol and beta-carotene), vitamin B (including folic acid and thiamine), vitamin C (ascorbic acid), vitamin D or vitamin E (including tocopherol) on fMRI signals or on cerebral blood flow.

There was also no literature dealing with amino acids generally, but tryptophan has been investigated because of its role as a serotonin precursor. Acute tryptophan depletion, as a way to manipulate serotonin levels, was found to modify regional BOLD responses to a cognitive task, but global effects weren't assessed. I was unable to find any literature investigating dietary glutamate (including as monosodium glutamate) or arginine.

Chronic ingestion of omega-3 fatty acids has been investigated using near-infrared spectroscopy, but only in frontal lobe where changes in both oxyhemoglobin and total hemoglobin were observed. Global measures of CBF weren't feasible. Taken together with prior fMRI results, which reported greater prefrontal BOLD signals in a sustained attention task without a change in task performance, it seems that some sort of vascular changes may be occurring. As with lipid ingestion generally, more work will be needed before we know whether there are general effects of concern for BOLD.

Foodstuffs high in flavonoids, including citrus and cocoa, are reputed to have cognitive and/or neuroprotective benefits. Consumption of a flavanone-rich drink was observed by Lamport et al. to produce "significantly increased regional perfusion in the inferior and middle right frontal gyrus at 2 h relative to baseline and the control drink. In addition, the [flavanone-rich] drink was associated with significantly improved performance on the Digit Symbol Substitution Test at 2 h relative to baseline and the control drink, but no effects were observed on any other behavioural cognitive tests." We might thus expect flavonoids in high doses to produce effects in fMRI experiments. Indeed, a study using chronic administration of blueberry juice observed both perfusion and fMRI signal changes in healthy eldery subjects. Would a typical dietary consumption be of concern? Unlike with caffeine, which may well be consumed in large amounts immediately before an fMRI experiment, it seems unlikely that subjects would be gorging on oranges or dark chocolate bars right before climbing in the scanner.  But if they do, you may want to know about it.


Diurnal factors and sleep


Those of you studying anxiety or the effects of sleep or sleep deprivation are presumably acutely aware of and controlling for potentially confounding physiologic changes in your study designs. The effects of intentional manipulation of cortisol and sleep deficiency on fMRI are well documented. What about the effects in "normal volunteers?" Stress (level of blood cortisol) and amount of sleep (drowsiness) may be factors for the time of day your study is conducted, and they may vary with the populations you sample from. (Ditto for caffeine use!) But I can't find any references specifically addressing these possibilities.  Keulers et al. measured cortisol levels for male adolescents in two age groups: 12-13 yrs and 16-17 yrs. They found: "Cortisol levels were enhanced pre-imaging relative to during and post-imaging in both age groups (but more in the older group), suggesting anticipatory stress and anxiety. Overall, a negative correlation was found between cortisol output during the fMRI experiment and brain activation magnitude during performance of a gambling task." These results were in line with previous research showing a reduction in anticipatory anxiety in adults undergoing fMRI. What is especially interesting is that the adolescents in the Keulers study had gone through training in a mock scanner a week before the real scan, begging the question as to whether the cortisol response is largely independent of experience. This would be important from a test-retest perspective. Peters et al. found that cortisol prior to an initial (real) scan was elevated but normalized before a second (real) scan, so perhaps the participants in the Keulers study knew not to be as apprehensive of the mock scanner. (Thanks to Toby Wise for the Peters et al. reference.)

In a comparison of drowsy and non-drowsy subjects, Poudel et al. found that acute sleep restriction - a single night with sleep restricted to 4 hrs - produced regional decreases in CBF, with the changes dependent on the level of drowsiness, but no change in global CBF between conditions. This suggests the patterns of CBF change are simply functions of underlying neural activity, which is good. However, drowsy subjects may tend to move more - like the nodding heads in a boring lecture - so you wouldn't necessarily want to scan undergraduates during finals week at 7 am unless you want high variability in your data.

There is also a now classic study which showed how inter-subject variability is much larger than intra-subject variability, so perhaps it's all baked in. Still, you would want to avoid biasing groups or longitudinal studies by the time of day you scan.


Hormones


(Cortisol is included in the section above, on diurnal factors.)

I was unable to find any papers dealing with concomitant physiological changes accompanying the menstrual cycle. It seems reasonable to expect that significant effects might be reflected in the heart rate, respiration rate/depth and blood pressure. Direct vasodilatory or vasoconstrictive effects seem unlikely to me, but I'm no endocrinologist. There are papers dealing with performance differences across menstrual cycle, but if these effects repeat they may have a neural basis rather than being physiological artifacts.

Women (and soon men?) going on or off hormonal contraceptives during a study might be a bigger deal. A recent study found a marked change in BOLD responses for women using oral contraception. The effects could be neural or concomitant physiological changes, or a mixture of both.

I couldn't leave this section without mentioning oxytocin. It's quite in vogue. I wasn't able to find any studies that had have investigated systemic physiological effects of this compound as they pertain to BOLD. But giving subjects oxytocin - or any other pharmacological agent come to that - without checking for concomitant physiological changes is taking a leap of faith that all effects are neural. At bare minimum I would expect blood pressure to be recorded with and without the challenge, and then I would want to see heart rate and respiration recorded during fMRI.

In a review available online just this week (16th Dec 2014), Quintana et al. state that "...(oxytocin) OT administration has been shown to increase (heart rate variability) HRV in both prairie voles (Grippo et al., 2009) and humans (Kemp et al., 2012 and Norman et al., 2011)." In the Norman et al. study, which used intranasal administration of oxytocin, they found no change in mean heart rate or blood pressure accompanying the HRV. However, according to the current Quintana review, when oxytocin is given intravenously there may be blood pressure effects: "In humans, reduced blood pressure has been observed in women administered OT during childbirth (Sartain et al., 2008 and Thomas et al., 2007) underscoring the role of peripheral OT in blood pressure regulation." The route of administration could thus be critical. According to an earlier Frontiers report by Quintana et al., "Research in humans suggests that intranasal administration of OT has no impact on HR (Norman et al., 2011; Kemp et al., 2012a,b). A lack of change in HR after OT administration may be due to intranasal OT increasing activity of both branches of the (autonomic nervous system) ANS (Norman et al., 2011)."

The onset and duration of oxytocin effects is also under scrutiny in the literature. To circumvent baseline drift issues that a long duration BOLD study would incur, Paloyelis et al. used arterial spin labeling (ASL) to measure both regional (rCBF) and global cerebral blood flow (CBF) in response to intranasal oxytocin. They found "...robust evidence delineating an oxytocinergic network comprising regions expected to express oxytocin receptors…" while "...rCBF maps indicated that intranasal oxytocin-induced changes were sustained over [...] 25–78 min and consistent with a pharmacodynamic profile showing a peak response at 39–51 min." There were some caveats, however: "We observed a general linear decrease over time in participants’ levels of alertness and excitement and in global CBF values in both groups... Self-reported levels of alertness and excitement correlated with global CBF values (alertness, r = .16, p = .009; excitement, r = .23, p < .001)." Hence, global CBF was included as a confounding covariate when analyzing regions-of-interest. That the global CBF decreased in both oxytocin and placebo groups suggests that all subjects' blood pressure decreased over time in the scanner (perhaps just from lying down, perhaps also from reduced stress from scanner familiarity - see the section on cortisol, above), but neither heart rate, blood pressure or respiration rates were reported. Collectively, the possibility of BP changes on oxytocin administration (at least for i.v. administration) coupled with the demonstrated changes in global CBF during a scan session, suggests that HR, BP and respiration should always be recorded in MRI studies, especially when giving pharmaceutical challenges.


Effects of the scanner itself


Almost everyone who has been in a high field MRI magnet is familiar with the sensation of vertigo, especially if you move your head too quickly. Many people also report getting a metallic taste in the mouth. What do we know about the physiologic consequences of being in a high field magnet? Recent research seems to have generated a pretty good understanding of the origins of vertigo. Roberts et al. (see also commentary by Straumann & Bockisch) noticed that their patients lacking labyrinthine function didn't experience nystagmus (rapid, involuntary eye movements) as did healthy subjects. They concluded that "...magnetic vestibular stimulation (MVS) derives from a Lorentz force resulting from interaction between the magnetic field and naturally occurring ionic currents in the labyrinthine endolymph fluid. This force pushes on the semicircular canal cupula, leading to nystagmus." Subsequent simulations by Antunes et al. and experiments at 7 T by Mian et al. were able to show that vertigo and nystagmus likely have a common mechanism. They also determined that the sensations tend to be transitory, abating over tens of seconds once head motion in the magnetic field is terminated. I haven't found a comparison of the variability of nystagmus with magnetic field strength for the same group of subjects, but nor is there evidence that scanning in 1.5 T or 3 T scanners presents difficulties for visual tasks or retinotopy, for example.

In my experience the metallic taste phenomenon isn't reported as often as vertigo but that may be because it's a more benign experience than feeling dizzy and disoriented. According to Cavin et al. it is movement through the magnetic field - that is, dB/dt - which produces the sensation. Moving slowly in the field reduces the sensation. They also report variable response across subjects and confirm that metallic fillings (or other dental hardware) are not the origin.

To date, nobody seems to have addressed explicitly the potential of confounding changes in BOLD-based fMRI arising out of either vertigo or the metallic taste. (I suppose we might better characterize these effects in terms of their (stimulus) interference potential rather than being BOLD confounds per se.) Several groups have assessed subjects' tolerance of these phenomena as well as their subjective perception, however, and these studies are probably a good place to start if you have reason to be concerned about an interaction between one or both phenomena and your experiment. Curiously, though, none of the studies I found looked specifically at the effects at 3 T. Is that because the effects are negligible - the positive results bias in the literature is getting a lot of attention recently - or is it because nobody has bothered to look yet? I have no idea. What I can tell you is that 7 T gets plenty of attention. Versluis et al. and Uwano et al. assessed with a questionnaire subjects' tolerance of a 7 T exam. Heilmaier et al. also used a questionnaire to assess subjects' perceptions, but in this case the subjects also underwent scanning at 1.5 T for comparison. Theysohn et al. assessed the effects of magnetic field on the vestibular system using a "sway path evaluation" measured via ultrasound transmitters mounted to the body of subjects who had recently exited a scanner (either 7 T, 1.5 T or 0 T) after 30 minute exposures. Temporary dysfunction was found for 7 T exposure whereas 1.5 T and 0 T had no effects.

The interplay between purely physiologic effects and the pyschological cascade arising from the stress of having a brain scan in a loud, dark, confined tube (and getting pictures of what we all hope is a normal, healthy brain at the end of it!) will undoubtedly be of concern for some fMRI experiments. Tandem experiments in a mock scanner is probably wise if there is concern over altered performance arising out of the magnetic field. Very recently, Ellerbrock & May showed that having an MRI scan increases pain perception. They interpret their findings in terms of awareness rather than being a physiologic phenomenon: "...we suggest that the scanning environment as such, being highly artificial and involving lying in a narrow and rather loud tube alerts the test persons consciously or unconsciously, leading to a higher awareness and thus to higher pain ratings." If so, one might think that a well-crafted mock scanner study would be able to find the same thing. And when considering the previously mentioned reduction of anticipatory anxiety for repeat MRI scans (see "Diurnal factors and sleep" section) these studies collectively suggest that it may be highly beneficial to train all subjects, adults as well as children, in mock scanners before doing fMRI experiments.

Age and disease


These variables should come as no major surprise if you are screening your subjects in any way at all. But in order to be complete and to give you a place to start if you're doing group studies across the lifespan, or comparing disease states to normal volunteers, I offer these papers:

Alterations in the BOLD fMRI signal with aging and disease: a challenge for neuroimaging.

Age-related differences in memory-encoding fMRI responses after accounting for decline in vascular reactivity.

Evidence that neurovascular coupling underlying the BOLD effect increases with age during childhood.

Age-related differences in cerebral blood flow underlie the BOLD fMRI signal in childhood.

Age dependence of hemodynamic response characteristics in human functional magnetic resonance imaging. 

Neural mechanisms of age-related slowing: the ΔCBF/ΔCMRO2 ratio mediates age-differences in BOLD signal and human performance.

Calibrated fMRI during a cognitive Stroop task reveals reduced metabolic response with increasing age.

Visualization of altered neurovascular coupling in chronic stroke patients using multimodal functional MRI.


Acceleration


If your subjects have just returned from a six-month stint on the International Space Station and you want to compare fMRI before and after the flight, you are no doubt fully aware of the many physiological factors to take into account where extended microgravity is concerned. Short duration microgravity - say, the few minutes to be experienced by lucky punters taking Virgin Galactic flights in the not-too-distant future - is probably not much of a concern, unless the flight happens the morning of their fMRI scan.

At the other end of the scale are those people exposing themselves to high acceleration; much greater than 1 g. Presumably these exposures are seconds or at most minutes in duration, but if the exposure occurs frequently or happens in the hours before participating in an fMRI experiment, we should be sure to exclude delayed physiologic changes. To date, though, I can find no reports detailing possible confounding effects of aerobatic plane flights, gymnastics, fairground rides, trampolining and the like. I am assuming these aren't going to be common issues for the vast majority of fMRI studies, but concomitant physiologic changes would be worth consideration if your research happens to involve fighter pilots, race car drivers, etc. Presumably measuring blood pressure, respiration and heart rates would capture the main effects. 


________________________



Notes:

1.  The other day someone told me about a subject who had answered negative to a drug screen but who then got very concerned when, on the morning of the scan, she was asked to give a urine sample. (I don't recall why the urine sample was required, and presumably it was stated on the recruitment flier which I can only assume the subject didn't read fully before applying to do the experiment. In any event, it was a vital part of the experiment and not a law-enforcement action!) She apparently returned from the bathroom with a sample of tap water in her specimen tube. The point? People will lie on screening forms if they are trying to make the $20 an hour you're offering. So if your experiment is highly sensitive to illicit drug use then you may want to include the option (or the threat?) of a urine test to help filter those people out.

2.  Hypoglycemia was also one of the conditions used (in rats) by Ogawa et al. in their preliminary demonstration of the BOLD effect. From the abstract of their 1990 paper: "This blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) contrast follows blood oxygen changes induced by anesthetics, by insulin-induced hypoglycemia, and by inhaled gas mixtures that alter metabolic demand or blood flow." So we're not exactly covering new ground here, are we?


2 comments:

  1. Nice work Ben, do you mind if I direct the students on my Intro to Neuroimaging course here to give them an overview of some potential confounds in BOLD imaging?

    Oh and on the topic of Hypoxia, you might want to check out this recent paper (shameless plug) on Hypoxic exposure that demonstrates heterogeneity in the the CBF response to Hypoxia.
    Lawley, J. S., Macdonald, J. H., Oliver, S. J., & Mullins, P. G. (2016). Unexpected reductions in regional cerebral perfusion during prolonged hypoxia. The Journal of Physiology. http://doi.org/10.1113/JP272557

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. Of course, send anyone this way! Hope they find it useful. I also have a more thorough review in prep but have been delayed getting a 1st draft out for a slew of reasons. Hopefully by the end of the year.
      Thanks for the article link, I'll read it closely and perhaps include it in the pertinent section in the new review paper. Cheers!

      Delete